Psychiatry Investig Search

CLOSE


Psychiatry Investig > Volume 14(5); 2017 > Article
Jeong, Jung, Kwak, Yang, Lim, Lee, Yoon, and Kim: Reliability and Validity of the Korean Version of the General-Food Craving Questionnaire-Trait for Children

Abstract

Objective

The General-Food Craving Questionnaire-Trait (G-FCQ-T) is a validated, assessment scale for food craving. The aim of this study was to measure its reliability and validity for Korean children.

Methods

A total of 172 children (94 boys and 78 girls) were selected to fill out a set of questionnaires, including the G-FCQ-T, the Children's version of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ-C), and the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) in the Korean language.

Results

The internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha=0.933) and test-retest reliability (r=0.653) were satisfactory. The G-FCQ-T showed a significantly positive correlation with the DEBQ-C (r=0.560) and the TFEQ (r=0.397). The optimum cutoff score of the G-FCQ-T set by Receiver Operating Characteristics analysis was 51, with sensitivity and specificity of 0.833 and 0.825, respectively, for children.

Conclusion

The G-FCQ-T showed good reliability and validity for assessing food craving for children and could become a practial instrument in clinical and research settings.

INTRODUCTION

“Craving” represents a strong motivational state and is defined in the dictionary as an intense desire or longing.1 In a similar vein, food craving is most often deemed as an intense urge to eat a certain food that is hard to stand up.2 Food craving is a matter of general experience among people and the prevalence of food craving ranges from 52% to 97% in previous reports.3
Several studies reported that food craving has been linked to future food consumption45 and a previous study demonstrated that high food cravers had stronger automatic approach inclinations for food compared to low food cravers.6 Furthermore, food craving is related to body weight2 and may be clinically associated with obesity.1 In obese people, it has been linked to the body mass index (BMI), the number of lifetime weight loss attempts, and the frequency of dropouts from obesity treatments.7 That is, obese or overweight adults show more food craving than normal or underweight adults do.8
Obesity in childhood is associated with adverse health consequences such as glucose intolerance and hyperlipidemia and predicts adult obesity.9 Although, it is still not well known whether food craving differs depending on age, a previous neuroimaging study reported that adolescents showed increased ventral striatum activation in response to food compared with adults.10 The ventral striatum is a part of the brain reward system and a few studies have indicated that heightened activity in this area was associated with the craving process.11 Another study also showed that children experience stronger craving, greater striatal recruitment, less prefrontal activity, and less frontostriatal coupling than adults.12 These findings imply that targeting subjective craving responses to food may be an important treatment element for subsequent weight gain and obesity.
However, people vary in the frequency, intensity, and types of food craving, so it is difficult to understand what exactly a food craving is. In the current conceptualization, food craving is assumed to be a complex phenomenon as it includes physiological, cognitive, emotional, and behavioral factors.21314
The most frequently used tools for assessing food craving are the Food Craving Questionnaires (FCQ).14 A trait of food craving that are consistent and long lasting characteristics across times and situations can be evaluated FCQ-Trait (FCQ-T).15 The FCQ-T is a self-report questionnaire, that was developed from above-mentioned viewpoint of the multidimensionality of food craving, and is sensitive to weight-loss treatment changes in obese individuals.16 The FCQ-T was later modified to measure a general index of trait food craving (General FCQ-T, G-FCQ-T) instead of a specific food craving.2
As previously mentioned, childhood is a sensitive period for the acquisition of obesity.17 To assess food craving in children could be useful for the investigation of the characteristics of children's general food consumption and cravings compared to adults and may be help to predict susceptibility to obesity including further adult obesity and weight-loss treatment success. There are a few instruments, such as the Children's version of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ-C) to measure children's eating styles that may contribute to or attenuate the development of overweight status.18 Nonetheless, to our knowledge, there is no instrument to evaluate children's eating behavior targeted especially at food craving. Therefore, the purposes of the present study were to evaluate the G-FCQ-T's reliability and validity for children while maintaining its basic structure.

METHODS

Study design and setting

This was a cross-sectional study of 5th grade students from an elementary school in Chung-ju city in South Korea. These students were approached from April 2015 to May 2015. Participants were 172 children (94 boys and 78 girls) with a mean age of 10.3 years (SD=0.5). This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Seoul St. Mary's Hospital (IRB number: KIRB-00512-21-003).

Procedure

Stage 1

The authors acquired the English version of the G-FCQ-T from Nijs et al.2 with permission. The G-FCQ-T scales were translated into Korean by two bilingual language psychiatrists, and modified to be adapted to young students, employing understandable sentences. Then a back translation was carried out by a third bilingual psychiatrist and the back-translated version was reviewed.

Stage 2

Each student and parent received an explanation of the purpose of the study. Parental consent was obtained for all participants. The students were administered the following questionnaires: the G-FCQ-T (1st G-FCQ-T), DEBQ-C, and Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ). The questionnaires were given under the supervision of a researcher and a teacher at the school. The researcher helped with reading if necessary.
In addition, weight and height were measured and BMI was calculated. To determine whether a student was underweight (BMI <5th percentile), normal weight (5th-84th percentile), overweight (85th-94th percentile), or obese (BMI ≥95th percentile), we used international cut-off scores.19

Stage 3

A month after completing the first set of questionnaires (1st G-FCQ-T, DEBQ-C, and TFEQ), the 172 students were requested to complete the G-FCQ-T (2nd G-FCQ-T) scale for a second time.

Instruments

G-FCQ-T2

The G-FCQ-T was developed to assess a general urge to eat and measures characteristics of food craving as a trait. The G-FCQ-T is a self-report scale with 21 items and each question has a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 to 6 (1=never to 6=always). The higher the total score is, the greater the degree of food craving. The scale contains four subdomains: 1) preoccupation with food, to assess obsessive thinking about food or eating (6 items), 2) loss of control, which is difficulty stopping eating when exposed to food cues (6 items), 3) positive outcome expectancy, to assess the extent of the expectation that eating will provide positive or negative reinforcement (5 items), and 4) emotional craving for food when experiencing negative emotions (4 items). The G-FCQ-T has demonstrated acceptable internal consistency with a Cronbach's α coefficient of 0.90 and construct validity in previous work.2

DEBQ-C18

The DEBQ-C is a 20-item measure of three types of eating behavior for children, namely emotional (7-items), external (6-items), and restrained eating (7-items). Items in the emotional eating subscale assess the proneness to overeat in response to negative emotional states. Items in the external eating subscale assess the proneness to overeat in response to food-related cues. Additionally, items in the restrained eating subscale assess the proneness to restrict food for weight control. The DEBQ-C allows three-choice responses of no (1), sometimes (2), or yes (3). In samples of 7- to 12-year-olds, Cronbach's alpha ranged from 0.73 to 0.82.18 In this study's sample, Cronbach's alpha was 0.862, 0.945, 0.828, and 0.886 for total DEBQ-C, emotional, external, and restrained eating, respectively.

TFEQ20

This scale is for prediction of eating behaviors. The TFEQ is made up 3 factors that assess a person's attitude toward eating. 1) Cognitive restraints of eating (21-items) reflects the tendency of some persons to cognitively control eating behavior and behaviorally restrain food intake in order to regulate their body weight. 2) Disinhibition of eating control (16-items) refers to personal differences in loss of inhibition of eating. 3) Susceptibility to hunger (14-items) reflects the subjective feeling of hunger and its behavioral consequences. The Cronbach's alpha was 0.820.21 In the current study, Cronbach's alpha was 0.809 for the total score and ranged from 0.621 to 0.803 for the subscales.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences version 21.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).
The internal consistencies of the G-FCQ-T and its subscales were evaluated by measuring Cronbach's alpha coefficients. The test-retest reliability was assessed by means of Pearson correlation coefficient.
Pearson's correlation was used to investigate the construct validity of the G-FCQ-T by examining relationships with the DEBQ-C and TFEQ.
The sensitivity, specificity, and area under the curve (AUC) were calculated and to find optimal cutoff points to predict high food cravers, receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis was applied to all participants dependent on the sum of emotional and external eating scales of DEBQ-C at the mean+2SD (standard deviation).
Exploratory principal component analysis with varimax rotation was performed on the G-FCQ-T.

RESULTS

Demographics and clinical characteristics (Table 1)

The percentage of people with 1) underweight, 2) normal weight, 3) overweight, and 4) obesity was 5.3, 64.9, 14.0, and 15.8%, respectively. No difference was seen in the BMI distribution between males and females. The mean total score of the 1st G-FCQ-T, DEBQ-C, TFEQ, and 2nd G-FCQ-T was 38.7, 31.2, 13.1, and 37.8, respectively. There were no gender differences in total or subdomain scores of 1st G-FCQ-T, DEBQ-C, TFEQ, and 2nd G-FCQ-T except for subscale scores of TFEQ cognitive control (12.9±3.7 in male, 14.0±4.3 in female, p<0.05) (data not shown). Only the DEBQ-C total (a, b<d) and DEBQ-C restrained eating subscale (a, b<c, d), TFEQ total (a, b<d), TFEQ cognitive restraints subscale (a, b<d), and TFEQ disinhibition subscale (b<d) scores showed significant differences between the BMI groups in the total or subdomain scores of the 1st G-FCQ-T, DEBQ-C, TFEQ, and 2nd G-FCQ-T (not shown).

Internal consistency and test-retest reliability (Table 2, Figure 1)

In 172 participants, the Cronbach's α coefficient was 0.933 for G-FCQ-T. The single item-total correlation coefficients ranged from 0.54 to 0.74 (Table 2). The test-retest reliability was measured after a 1-month interval. Cronbach's α after a 1-month interval was 0.925. The correlation coefficient of the G-FCQ-T total scores in the 2 trials was 0.653 and showed good test-retest reliability (p<0.001) (Figure 1).

Construct validity (Table 3)

To assess construct validity, we compared the G-FCQ-T with the DEBQ-C and TFEQ. The correlation coefficients were 0.560 (p<0.01) and 0.397 (p<0.01) for G-FCQ-T total score with DEBQ-C and TFEQ total score, respectively. There were more considerable correlations between G-FCQ-T and DEBQ-C scores than between G-FCQ-T and TFEQ scores. The G-FCQ-T total and all 4 subdomain scores were related to the DEBQ-C emotional eating and external eating subscale scores, but no significant correlation between the G-FCQ-T and DEBQ-C restrained eating subscale was found. Further, G-FCQ-T scores were also related to the TFEQ disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger subscales, but no significant correlation between the G-FCQ-T and TFEQ cognitive restraints subscale was found. In addition, the strongest correlations were shown between the DEBQ-C emotional eating and G-FCQ-T emotional craving subscale, and between the DEBQ-C external eating and G-FCQ-T loss of control subscale.

ROC analysis (Table 4, Figure 2)

ROC analysis was applied to predict food craving. Sensitivity and specificity were calculated for G-FCQ-T scores relative to the sum of emotional and external eating scales of DEBQ-C at the mean+2SD. The DEBQ-C was constructed using children18 and previous studies demonstrated that food cravers had significantly elevated external eating and emotional eating subscale scores on the DEBQ.2223 The AUC value was 0.917 (0.856-0.978), the cut-off value was 51, sensitivity value was 0.833, and specificity value was 0.825. Applying the given cut-off point in the present sample, food cravers accounted for about 20% of the sample (n=34).

Exploratory factor analysis (Table 5)

The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure showed a value of 0.909 and Bartlett's test of sphericity was significant (p<0.001). The principal-components factor analysis on the items from the G-FCQ-T for all 172 subjects revealed that there were four factors that explained 65.61% of the total variance. Factor I explained 45.63% of the variance (eigenvalue, 9.58) and was construed as loss of control. Factor II (eigenvalue, 1.68) loaded on preoccupation with food. Factor III (eigenvalue, 1.48) loaded on positive outcome expectancy. Factor IV (eigenvalue, 1.04) loaded on negative emotional craving. However, question (Q) number 3 (Q3), Q8, and Q9 were classified in preoccupation with food, Q6 in positive outcome expectancy, and Q16 in negative emotional craving in the original version.

DISCUSSION

The current results suggest that the G-FCQ-T is a valid and reliable instrument for measuring food craving for children. The internal consistency and test-retest reliability were satisfactory. The results of comparing the G-FCQ-T with DEBQ-C or TFEQ also showed good validity and significant positive correlations between the G-FCQ-T and DEBQ-C or TFEQ scores. The optimum cutoff score of G-FCQ-T set by ROC analysis was 51 in children.
Consistent with our results, previous studies with young adults showed high internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha=0.875-0.90) and two- or three-week test-retest reliability (r=0.79-0.850) for the G-FCQ-T.221 Additionally, they showed significant positive correlations between the G-FCQ-T and adult version of the DEBQ (r=0.23-0.492) or TFEQ (r=0.435).221 These results implied that the G-FCQ-T has good psychometric properties for the assessment of food craving both in children and adults.
Furthermore, the present study also demonstrated the associations between the G-FCQ-T and DEBQ-C or TFEQ subscales. Significant positive correlations were shown between the G-FCQ-T scales and DEBQ-C emotional or external eating, and between the G-FCQ-T scales and the TFEQ disinhibition or susceptibility to hunger subscales. These subscales reflect a tendency toward disinhibited eating occurance along with food exposure, negative feelings, or subjective feelings of hunger. Although the DEBQ-C and TFEQ are measurements to assess not food craving but eating behavior, previous studies have shown associations between external, emotional eating, disinhibition, and susceptibility to hunger with strength and frequency of food craving similar to our results.32224 That is the reason why the present authors deemed participants with scores exceeding the mean plus two standard deviations of the sum scores on the DEBQ-C emotional and external eating to be eligible for the group of high food cravers in the ROC analysis. We only used the DEBQ-C scores as a standard as it is a children-adapted scale, rather than the TFEQ ones. The cutoff score of the G-FCQ-T in the present study (a total score of 51) is a stricter criterion than that of another study6 that used scoring in the upper third (about a total score of 40 in the present study) to predict food craving.
According to exploratory factor analysis, the G-FCQ-T had a four-factor structure; the present authors interpreted the four factors as indicative of preoccupation with food, loss of control, positive outcome expectancy, and emotional craving as in the original version. However, it was not possible to reproduce the original factors of preoccupation with food and loss of control (Q 3, 5, and 17). These factors contain a mixture of questions on uncontrolled eating behavior, connected with thoughts or preoccupation with food. The reason for failure to replicate these two factors may be related to age differences of the participants. The original version of G-FCQ-T was developed in a sample of college students. An earlier study reported that thoughts preoccupied with food or craving and uncontrolled food consumption are tightly coupled in childhood but become less so with age.12 This finding implies that the link between preoccupation with food and lack of control over eating in children can be attributed to this mixed result in scale construction. In the case of Q6 and Q16 of the G-FCQ-T in our sample, the translation of these items might have underrepresented the meaning of how positive outcome expectancy or negative emotion influence food craving, and this may also have affected the scale construction. Therefore, further validation work in larger and independent samples of children is required.
Past research indicates that gender differences may be present in the characteristics of food craving, such as prevalence, affective state, or food selection during food craving.714 Compared to men, women were more likely to experience food craving and reported more emotional eating, and these differences were assumed to be related to hormonal differences.7 In addition, another study showed a significant positive relationship between BMI and food craving25 and food-craving responses to food stimuli were related to measures of insulin resistance and thalamic brain activation in individuals with obesity compared to those of normal weight.1 In contrast to earlier findings, however, the results of the present study showed no differences in food craving between gender and BMI groups. These inconsistencies may be due to the ages of participants, since in our study, all participants were children, including premenarcheal girls.
This study has the following limitations. First, participants were limited to 5th grade elementary school students from one school of a specific region. Prior studies have noted developmental comfort food preferences26 and developmental changes in functional brain responses to images of foods.27 Second, we did not screen for psychiatric disorders especially eating disorders, or assess which phase of the menstrual cycle the female students were in. Several reports have shown that food craving was followed by binge eating,28 one of the features of eating disorders, and that menstrual cycle effects on eating occur independently.2930 Therefore, the results need to be replicated in samples with various age groups, particularly adolescents, and in individuals with eating disorders. Third, the G-FCQ-T was developed in a sample of university students, not children. Although the present authors tried to modify the scale to adapt it to young students, and used the children's version of the DEBQ to evaluate construct validity, the original version of the G-FCQ-T was validated with young adults. Fourth, the DEBQ-C and TFEQ were not validated among Korean children or adolescents. However, the DEBQ-C and TFEQ were validated among 473 Spanish children aged 10 to 14 years31 and 769 Dutch children aged 7 to 12 years,18 and among 192 Spanish children aged 8.8 to 16.8 years,32 respectively. Fifth, DEBQ-C scores, instead of a clinical diagnosis of food craving, were used in the ROC analysis because of the difficulty of confirming craving and lack of consensus about the definition of craving. Nevertheless, a cutoff point was obtained by applying relatively strict criteria, and this point may be useful to inform preliminary research and prompt decision to assess food craving. Sixth, we examined the test-retest reliability by performing the retest after a 1-month interval. Even though the time between the test and re-test in the present study was longer than that used in previous studies,221 we could not exclude the attribution of the learning effect by the participants. A faster learning rate has been reported when the retest time interval decreases.33 Therefore, longer test-retest intervals could reflect better test-retest reliability.
In spite of these limitations, this study suggests that the G-FCQ-T has satisfactory psychometric properties in children. That is, the G-FCQ-T is a reliable and valid tool for assessing craving in children in Korea. Furthermore, this study also provided a cutoff score, which may assist clinicians in screening for high food cravers in children. When considering that childhood is a sensitive period for the acquisition of obesity and the association between food craving and obesity, this scale and cutoff point will provide new insights to approach the management of and predict susceptibility to overeating and obesity in children. More research is required to evaluate the G-FCQ-T, including confirmatory factor analysis and studies applying to a wide range of age groups.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Brain Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT & Future Planning (NRF-2013M3C8A2A02078508).

References

1. Potenza MN, Grilo CM. How relevant is food craving to obesity and its treatment? Front Psychiatry 2014;5:164PMID: 25477827.
crossref pmid pmc
2. Nijs IM, Franken IH, Muris P. The modified trait and state food-cravings questionnaires: development and validation of a general index of food craving. Appetite 2007;49:38-46. PMID: 17187897.
crossref pmid
3. Gilhooly CH, Das SK, Golden JK, McCrory MA, Dallal GE, Saltzman E, et al. Food cravings and energy regulation: the characteristics of craved foods and their relationship with eating behaviors and weight change during 6 months of dietary energy restriction. Int J Obes (Lond) 2007;31:1849-1858. PMID: 17593902.
crossref pmid
4. Flint A, Raben A, Blundell JE, Astrup A. Reproducibility, power and validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single test meal studies. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2000;24:38-48. PMID: 10702749.
crossref pmid pdf
5. Martin CK, O'Neil PM, Tollefson G, Greenway FL, White MA. The association between food cravings and consumption of specific foods in a laboratory taste test. Appetite 2008;51:324-326. PMID: 18417253.
crossref pmid pmc
6. Brockmeyer T, Hahn C, Reetz C, Schmidt U, Friederich HC. Approach bias and cue reactivity towards food in people with high versus low levels of food craving. Appetite 2015;95:197-202. PMID: 26184338.
crossref pmid
7. Imperatori C, Innamorati M, Tamburello S, Continisio M, Contardi A, Tamburello A, et al. Gender differences in food craving among overweight and obese patients attending low energy diet therapy: a matched case-control study. Eat Weight Disord 2013;18:297-303. PMID: 23904055.
crossref pmid
8. Delahanty LM, Meigs JB, Hayden D, Williamson DA, Nathan DM. Diabetes Prevenion Program (DPP) Research Group. Psychological and behavioral correlates of baseline BMI in the diabetes prevention program (DPP). Diabetes Care 2002;25:1992-1998. PMID: 12401745.
crossref pmid pmc
9. Burrows T, Meule A. ‘Food addiction’. What happens in childhood? Appetite 2015;89:298-300. PMID: 25543074.
crossref pmid
10. Galvan A, McGlennen KM. Enhanced striatal sensitivity to aversive reinforcement in adolescents versus adults. J Cogn Neurosci 2013;25:284-296. PMID: 23163417.
crossref pmid
11. Kober H, Mende-Siedlecki P, Kross EF, Weber J, Mischel W, Hart CL, et al. Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2010;107:14811-14816. PMID: 20679212.
crossref pmid pmc
12. Silvers JA, Insel C, Powers A, Franz P, Weber J, Mischel W, et al. Curbing craving: behavioral and brain evidence that children regulate craving when instructed to do so but have higher baseline craving than adults. Psychol Sci 2014;25:1932-1942. PMID: 25193941.
crossref pmid pmc
13. Nederkoorn C, Smulders FT, Jansen A. Cephalic phase responses, craving and food intake in normal subjects. Appetite 2000;35:45-55. PMID: 10896760.
crossref pmid
14. Rodriguez-Martin BC, Meule A. Food craving: new contributions on its assessment, moderators, and consequences. Front Psychol 2015;6:21PMID: 25657636.
crossref pmid pmc
15. Cepeda-Benito A, Gleaves DH, Williams TL, Erath SA. The development and validation of the state and trait food-cravings questionnaires. Behav Ther 2000;31:151-173.
crossref
16. Batra P, Das SK, Salinardi T, Robinson L, Saltzman E, Scott T, et al. Relationship of cravings with weight loss and hunger. Results from a 6 month worksite weight loss intervention. Appetite 2013;69:1-7. PMID: 23684901.
crossref pmid
17. Dietz WH. Critical periods in childhood for the development of obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 1994;59:955-959. PMID: 8172099.
crossref pmid
18. van Strien T, Oosterveld P. The children's DEBQ for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating in 7- to 12-year-old children. Int J Eat Disord 2008;41:72-81. PMID: 17634965.
crossref pmid
19. Cole TJ, Bellizzi MC, Flegal KM, Dietz WH. Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: international survey. BMJ 2000;320:1240-1243. PMID: 10797032.
crossref pmid pmc
20. Stunkard AJ, Messick S. The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res 1985;29:71-83. PMID: 3981480.
crossref pmid
21. Noh JH, Kim JH, Nam HJ, Lim MR, Lee DS, Hong KS. Validation of the Korean version of the General Food Cravings Questionnaire-Trait (G-FCQ-T). Korean J Clin Psychology 2008;27:1039-1051.
crossref
22. Hill AJ, Weaver CF, Blundell JE. Food craving, dietary restraint and mood. Appetite 1991;17:187-197. PMID: 1799281.
crossref pmid
23. Tuomisto T, Hetherington MM, Morris MF, Tuomisto MT, Turjanmaa V, Lappalainen R. Psychological and physiological characteristics of sweet food “addiction”. Int J Eat Disord 1999;25:169-175. PMID: 10065394.
crossref pmid
24. Rogers PJ, Smit HJ. Food craving and food “addiction”: a critical review of the evidence from a biopsychosocial perspective. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2000;66:3-14. PMID: 10837838.
crossref pmid
25. Chao A, Grilo CM, White MA, Sinha R. Food cravings, food intake, and weight status in a community-based sample. Eat Behav 2014;15:478-482. PMID: 25064302.
crossref pmid pmc
26. Wansink B, Cheney MM, Chan N. Exploring comfort food preferences across age and gender. Physiol Behav 2003;79:739-747. PMID: 12954417.
crossref pmid
27. Killgore WD, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Developmental changes in the functional brain responses of adolescents to images of high and low-calorie foods. Dev Psychobiol 2005;47:377-397. PMID: 16284969.
crossref pmid
28. Waters A, Hill A, Waller G. Bulimics' responses to food cravings: is binge-eating a product of hunger or emotional state? Behav Res Ther 2001;39:877-886. PMID: 11480829.
crossref pmid
29. Cohen IT, Sherwin BB, Fleming AS. Food cravings, mood, and the menstrual cycle. Horm Behav 1987;21:457-470. PMID: 3428887.
crossref pmid
30. Bancroft J, Cook A, Williamson L. Food craving, mood and the menstrual cycle. Psychol Med 1988;18:855-860. PMID: 3270830.
crossref pmid
31. Banos RM, Cebolla A, Etchemendy E, Felipe S, Rasal P, Botella C. Validation of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire for Children (DEBQ-C) for use with Spanish children. Nutr Hosp 2011;26:890-898. PMID: 22470039.
pmid
32. Martín-García M, Vila-Maldonado S, Rodríguez-Gómez I, Faya FM, Plaza-Carmona M, Pastor-Vicedo JC, et al. The Spanish version of the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire-R21 for children and adolescents (TFEQ-R21C): psychometric analysis and relationships with body composition and fitness variables. Physiol Behav 2016;165:350-357. PMID: 27538345.
crossref pmid
33. Lemay S, Bedard MA, Rouleau I, Tremblay PL. Practice effect and test-retest reliability of attentional and executive tests in middle-aged to elderly subjects. Clin Neuropsychol 2004;18:284-302. PMID: 15587675.
crossref pmid
Figure 1

Scatterplot showing the correlation between scores on the G-FCQ-T between the two measurements (r=0.653, p<0.01). G-FCQ-T: General-Food Craving Questionnaire-Trait.

pi-14-595-g001.jpg
Figure 2

The ROC curve for the G-FCQ-T to predict food craving. ROC: receiver operating characteristic, G-FCQ-T: General-Food Craving Questionnaire-Trait.

pi-14-595-g002.jpg
Table 1

Characteristics of the study population (N=172)

pi-14-595-i001.jpg

G-FCQ-T: General-Food Craving Questionnaire-Trait, DEBQ-C: Children's version of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire, TFEQ: Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire

Table 2

Corrected item-total correlations and Cronbach's alpha if item was deleted for the G-FCQ-T

pi-14-595-i002.jpg

G-FCQ-T: General-Food Craving Questionnaire-Trait

Table 3

Construct validity of G-FCQ-T (Pearson's correlation): subscales of the G-FCQ-T and the DEBQ-C and TFEQ

pi-14-595-i003.jpg

*p<0.01. G-FCQ-T: General-Food Craving Questionnaire-Trait, DEBQ-C: Children's version of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire, TFEQ: Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire

Table 4

Summary of ROC analysis predicting food craving

pi-14-595-i004.jpg

AUC: area under the curve, CI: confidence interval, ROC: receiver operating characteristic

Table 5

Exploratory factor analysis of the G-FCQ-T

pi-14-595-i005.jpg

Rotation method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. Highest loadings, on the basis of which items are allocated to a factor, are printed in bold. G-FCQ-T: General-Food Craving Questionnaire-Trait



ABOUT
AUTHOR INFORMATION
ARTICLE CATEGORY

Browse all articles >

BROWSE ARTICLES
Editorial Office
#522, 27, Seochojungang-ro 24-gil, Seocho-gu, Seoul 06601, Korea
Tel: +82-2-717-0892    E-mail: psychiatryinvest@gmail.com                

Copyright © 2024 by Korean Neuropsychiatric Association.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer
prev next