The Relationship between Social Network Service Use Motives and Subjective Well-Being: The Mediating Effect of Online and Offline Social Capital
Article information
Abstract
Objective
The purpose of this study was to examine the mediating effect of social capital on the relationship between Social Media use motives and subjective well-being.
Methods
In the study, online self-reporting surveys were conducted with Social Media users in their 20s, and data from 445 participants were used for structural equation modeling.
Results
The main findings of the study were as follows. First, the interpersonal motives for Social Media use had an indirect effect on subjective well-being by mediating offline bonding capital and online bonding capital. In addition, interpersonal motives had an indirect effect on subjective well-being by dual-mediating online and offline bonding capital. Second, the self-expression motive for Social Media use did not directly affect subjective well-being, but it indirectly affected subjective well-being by mediating offline bonding capital. Third, the information-seeking motive for Social Media use did not directly affect subjective well-being, but it indirectly affected subjective well-being by mediating offline bonding capital.
Conclusion
This study identified a specific mechanism for how motives for using Social Media affect subjective well-being. Furthermore, the results of this study suggest that the effect of Social Media use on subjective well-being may differ depending on the motive for Social Media use.
INTRODUCTION
As of 2018, the number of Social Media users worldwide is estimated to be more than 2.95 billion, which is about one-third of the world’s total population [1]. According to the Korea Media Panel Survey [2], only 16.8% of all respondents used Social Media in 2011, but 47.7% of all respondents used Social Media in 2019. Furthermore, according to “2020 Survey on the internet Usage” in Korea, the social network service (SNS) availability ratio by age group was high in the order of those in their 20s, 30s, and 40s, and the SNS availability ratio in their 20s was the highest [3]. Early studies on Social Media focused on the negative effects of Social Media use such as emotional and interpersonal problems arising from the excessive use of Social Media [4-6]. However, it is worth noting that the majority of Social Media users do not complain of psychological or emotional problems and use Social Media in their daily lives.
Previous studies found that Social Media not only helps build and maintain relationships with others but also helps expand relationships with strangers and different groups [7,8]. The intimacy and social support that can be obtained through Social Media activities can provide a healthy and happy life [9,10]. In fact, in the 18–34-year-old group that uses Social Media a lot, the number of friends in Social Media was related to increased social support and subjective well-being [11].
Studies on the relationship between Social Media use and subjective well-being show contradictory results. According to the use and gratification theory, individuals who use the media select and use the media that satisfies their motives and needs, and the impact of media use on an individual’s psychological and emotional state can vary depending on the motivations (or purposes) with which they use the media [12,13].
Social Media use motive is used interchangeably with terms such as purpose of Social Media use and type of Social Media use. Social Media use motive is a concept related to the purposes for which users use Social Media (e.g., personal relationships, self-expression, information seeking, etc.). The use and gratification theory assumes that the way or purpose of media use reflects the user’s needs to be satisfied through the media [14]. Based on this theory, this study used the term Social Media use motive. Based on past studies, we classified the Social Media use motives as “interpersonal motive” to maintain and shape relationship, “self-expression motive” to express one’s thoughts, and “information-seeking motive” to explore or share information [15-17].
The main motive for Social Media use is interaction with others. The interpersonal motive for Social Media use has been reported to have a positive effect on subjective well-being [18]. The interpersonal motive for Social Media use may reduce depressed mood and increase mutual trust, which, in turn, can increase subjective well-being [19]. Self-expression and information-seeking motives for Social Media use have also been reported to have a positive effect on subjective well-being [20-23].
Since Social Media can establish various relationships through online networks and enable real-time communication and information sharing, the Social Media use motive can influence social capital. Social capital refers to resources that can be obtained through various social relationships [24]. Social capital theory explains that individuals can form trust, reciprocity, and norms among members through social networks [25]. Social capital can be categorized into bonding social capital and bridging social capital. Bonding social capital is a resource provided between individuals with strong ties, such as close friends and family, while bridging social capital is a resource provided between individuals or groups with weak ties, such as acquaintances or strangers [25,26]. In particular, Williams [26] emphasized that it is necessary to distinguish between online and offline social capital and divided social capital into online bonding social capital, online bridging social capital, offline bonding social capital, and offline bridging social capital.
Researchers’ views on the effect of Social Media use on social capital are largely divided into two perspectives. First, the use of Social Media will have a positive effect on the formation of social capital. Scholars holding this view argue that online interactions increase interpersonal opportunities and, consequently, can improve the quality of relationships [27]. In addition, they assume that online interactions foster communication and trust by strengthening connections between people with common interests [28]. Second, the use of Social Media will have a negative effect on the formation of social capital. Scholars who hold this view argue that the excessive use of Social Media simplifies human relationships and impedes true ties or unity [29,30]. In addition to the two perspectives, the view that the use of Social Media plays a role in maintaining and supplementing the existing social ties emerged. This view considers online relationships as an extension of offline relationships and argues that Social Media use can help maintain existing relationships [7].
Bargh and McKenna [8] argued that an intimate relationship formed online can affect offline relationships, and Kim [9] said that reciprocal interactions obtained online can help solve offline relationship problems. It means that reciprocal and reliable relationships acquired through Social Media can influence offline bonding social capital.
Previous studies have explored the relationship between Social Media use motives and social capital. The results indicate that the interpersonal motive and information-seeking motive for Social Media use have a positive effect on social capital [31], and social activities such as interpersonal interactions and information search have a positive effect on both bonding social capital and bridging social capital [32]. In addition, self-expression in Social Media can increase interpersonal connectivity [33], and the self-expression motive for Social Media use has been reported to increase the positive feedback of people, which has a positive effect on both bonding and bridging social capital [34].
Social capital was also found to be closely related to subjective well-being. According to previous studies, positive factors such as trust and volunteering of social capital have a positive effect on subjective well-being [35]. In addition, many past studies have confirmed the relationship between social capital and subjective well-being [7,36,37].
The impact of social capital on subjective well-being may vary depending on the type of social capital. It has been reported that social capital composed of intimate and close relationships, such as bonding social capital, can provide more psychological rewards to users and consequently positively affect the quality of life [38,39]. In a study by Pang [37] bonding social capital had a positive effect on subjective well-being, but bridging social capital did not significantly affect subjective well-being.
Considered together, the effect of Social Media use on subjective well-being may vary depending on Social Media use motives, and an effort is needed to understand the specific mechanisms underlying how Social Media use motives affect subjective well-being. Therefore, this study analyzes the effect of each Social Media use motive on subjective well-being and examines the mediating effects of social capital in the relationship between Social Media use motives and subjective well-being. The hypotheses of this study were as follows.
Hypothesis 1: the Social Media use motives will have a positive impact on subjective well-being. The Social Media use motives will affect subjective well-being, and it is thought that there will be a difference in type depending on each motive (interpersonal, self-expression, and information seeking).
Hypothesis 2: the Social Media use motives will have a positive impact on social capital. The Social Media use motives are expected to affect the formation of social capital. However, it is thought that there is a difference in the social capital (online bonding, online bridging, offline bonding, and offline bridging) formed by each motive (interpersonal, self-expression, and information seeking).
Hypothesis 3: there will be a mediating effect of social capital in the relationship between the Social Media use motives and subjective well-being. In the relationship between Social Media use motives and subjective well-being, the mediating effect of bonding social capital (online bonding and offline bonding) rather than bridging social capital (online bridging and offline bridging) is expected.
METHODS
Participants
In this study, an online self-reporting survey (Google questionnaire form) was conducted on Social Media users living in Gyeonggi-do, Chungcheong-do, Gyeongsang-do, and other regions. The participants were recruited through Social Media and online communities, and the survey was conducted from June 11, 2019 to June 21, 2019. It included 476 participants in the survey. Data from 445 participants were used in the final analysis, as 31 respondents either did not meet the age criteria of the study or did not respond to more than 50% of the items. They were judged to have responded unfaithfully. Of the 445 participants, 208 (46.7%) were male and 237 (53.3%) were female, and the average age was 21.8 (standard deviation=0.81) years. The regional distribution of Social Media users was as follows: 247 (55.5%) in Gyeonggi-do, 108 (24.3%) in Gyeongsang-do, 71 (15.9%) in Chungcheong-do, and 19 (4.3%) in other regions. The types of Social Media mainly used were as follows: 246 (55.3%) Instagram, 170 (38.2%) Facebook, 22 (4.9%) Twitter, 5 (1.1%) Kakao Stories, and 2 Naver Bands (0.5%). Specific demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1. All research procedures, including data collection, were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Dankook University (IRB number: 2019-09-034).
Measures
Social Media use motive
In this study, Social Media use motive was classified into interpersonal motive, self-expression motive, and information-seeking motive. The scale is composed of 26 questions, including eight questions on interpersonal motive [40-42], nine questions on self-expression motive [43,44], and nine questions of information-seeking motive [40,41,43]. Each item consisted of a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Examples of items from each subscale are as follows. 1) Self-expression motive=“to show one’s personality or hobbies,” “to post my pictures or writings.” 2) Information-seeking motive=“to find various information,” “to get opinions on various topics.” 3) Interpersonal motive=“to keep in touch with an acquaintance,” “to talk to others.” An exploratory factorial analysis was conducted to verify the validity of the scale. The maximum likelihood method was used for factor extraction, and the direct oblimin method was applied for factor rotation. As a result of the exploratory factor analysis, a total of 17 items were selected, with five questions on interpersonal motive, six questions on self-expression motive, and six questions on information-seeking motive. Table 2 shows the results of the factor analysis. The eigenvalues of the factors were 6.04, 3.04, and 2.43, respectively, and the total cumulative explanatory variance was 67.74%. In this study, Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was 0.81 for interpersonal motive, 0.92 for self-expression motive, and 0.84 for information-seeking motive.
Social capital
In this study, the translated and modified version of Williams [26] was used to measure social capital [45]. This scale is composed of 10 questions on online bonding social capital, 10 questions on online bridging social capital, 10 questions on offline bonding social capital, and 10 questions on offline bridging social capital. Each item consists of a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Examples of items from each subscale are as follows. 1) Online bonding social capital=“there is someone online who I am sure can help solve my problem,” “there is someone online who can advise you on a very important decision.” 2) Online bridging social capital=“through the person I met online, I became interested in people who thought differently from me,” “meeting people online is a great opportunity to talk to new people.” 3) Offline bonding social capital=“there is someone offline who I am sure can help solve my problem,” “there is someone offline who can advise you on a very important decision.” 4) Offline bridging social capital=“through the person I met offline, I became interested in people who thought differently from me,” “meeting people offline is a great opportunity to talk to new people.” In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha of the scale was 0.84 for online bonding social capital, 0.93 for online bridging social capital, 0.94 for offline bonding social capital, and 0.92 for offline bridging social capital.
Subjective well-being
In this study, a Concise Measure of Subjective Well-Being developed by Suh and Koo [46] was used to measure subjective well-being. This scale is composed of nine questions, including three questions on life satisfaction that mean satisfaction with one’s life, three questions on positive emotion experience that asked about positive emotions felt in the past month, and three questions on negative emotion experience that asked about negative emotions felt in the past month. The items about life satisfaction consisted of a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), and items on positive and negative emotions consisted of a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (always). The Cronbach’s alpha of the scale in this study was 0.85 for life satisfaction, 0.85 for positive emotion experience, and 0.72 for negative emotion experience.
Covariates
Age, sex, and types of Social Media use were added as covariates in this study. Participants were asked to choose the social media platform you use the most between Instagram, Facebook, Twitter, Kakao Story, Naver Band.
Analysis
In this study, IBM SPSS Statistics Version 24.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) and AMOS 24.0 (IBM SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA) were used to analyze the collected data. A descriptive statistics analysis was conducted to identify the mean and standard deviation of the major variables. A Pearson correlation analysis was performed to verify the correlation between the major variables. The model fit was tested using the structural equation model, and the significance of the indirect effect was verified using bootstrapping. Finally, before verifying the research model, observed variables were constructed through item parceling. In the case of subjective well-being that has subscale, the content-based approach was used to consist of three observed variables: life satisfaction, positive emotion experience, and negative emotion experience. Online bonding social capital, online bridging social capital, offline bonding social capital, and offline bridging social capital which are single factors, the factorial algorithm was used to consist of three observed variables [47]. And a multiple mediator model was established to check the indirect effects of social capital subfactors between Social Media use motives and subjective well-being. Phantom variables were set to verify the indirect effects of subfactors of social capital [48]. The model’s goodness of fit was verified by comprehensively considering and various fit indices [49]. The fit of the research model is considered good, if Comparative fit Index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) were greater than 0.9, and root meansquare residual (RMSEA) was less than 0.05.
RESULTS
Descriptive statistics and correlations
The results of descriptive statistics and correlations are presented in Tables 3 and 4. Among the Social Media use motives, interpersonal motive positively correlated with subjective well-being (r=0.111, p<0.05). In relationship with social capital, interpersonal motive positively correlated with online bonding (r=0.505, p<0.01) and online bridging social capital (r=0.501, p<0.01), and negatively correlated with offline bonding social capital (r=-0.107, p<0.05). Self-expression motive positively correlated with online bonding (r=0.368, p<0.01) and online bridging social capital (r=0.429, p<0.01), and information-seeking motive positively correlated with online bonding (r=0.216, p<0.01), online bridging (r=0.239, p<0.01), and offline bridging social capital (r=0.185, p<0.01). In the relationship between social capital and subjective well-being, online bonding (r=0.131, p<0.01), offline bonding (r=0.214, p<0.01), and offline bridging social capital (r=0.216, p<0.001) positively correlated with subjective well-being. Multivariate normality was secured because the skewness value did not exceed 2 and the kurtosis value did not exceed 7.
Structural equation model
Confirmatory factor analysis
Before verifying the research model, a confirmatory factor analysis of variables was conducted after using item parceling to consider global model fit and estimation stability [50]. The verification results of the measurement model are presented in Table 5. It was found that the model fit was good for all the measurement variables (chi-square=657.324, p>0.05, df=322, CFI=0.956, TLI=0.948, RMSEA=0.048). This result means that the measurement variables properly measured the construct.
Structural equation modeling
The path between self-expression motive and subjective well-being, and the path between information-seeking motive and subjective well-being were deleted from the final model because the correlation between the variables was not significant.
The analysis of the research model showed the following: chi-square=1,068.995, p<0.05, df=531, CFI=0.946, TLI=0.940, RMSEA=0.048. The research model is presented in Figure 1.
In this study, an alternative model was established by adding a path from online binding capital to offline binding capital. The alternative model was a nested model of the research model, and the two models were compared using the chi-square difference test presented by Bentler and Bonett [51]. The result showed that the fit of model was good (chi-square=1,057.498, p<0.05, df=530, CFI=0.947, TLI=0.941, RMSEA=0.047). In addition, it was found that the chi-square value of the alternative model decreased by more than 3.84 compared to the decrease of the degree of freedom by 1 in the research model (χ2=11.497), which was determined to be more suitable for the alternative model than the research model at a significance level of 0.05. Thus, the alternative models (Figure 2) were determined as the final model.
The results of the analysis of the pathways are presented in Table 6. It was found that interpersonal motive did not significantly affect subjective well-being.
Online bonding social capital (β=0.29, p<0.01) and offline bonding social capital (β=0.23, p<0.05) had a positive effect on subjective well-being, but online bridging social capital (β=-0.21, p<0.05) had a negative effect on subjective well-being and offline bridging social capital did not significantly affect subjective well-being. These results indicate that the more the bonding social capital, the more the subjective well-being increases.
Interpersonal motive had a positive effect on online bonding social capital (β=0.68, p<0.001) and online bridging social capital (β=0.53, p<0.001) but had a negative effect on offline bonding social capital (β=-0.56, p<0.001) and offline bridging social capital (β=-0.28, p<0.01). Self-expression motive had a positive effect on the online bridging social capital (β=0.14, p<0.05), offline bonding social capital (β=0.26, p<0.001), and offline bridging social capital (β=0.21, p<0.01) but did not affect online bonding social capital. Information-seeking motive had a positive effect on offline bonding social capital (β=0.24, p<0.001) and offline bridging social capital (β=0.28, p<0.001), but it did not affect online bonding social capital and online bridging social capital.
Mediating effect of social capital
A bootstrapping test was conducted to verify the mediating effect of social capital on the relationship between Social Media use motives and subjective well-being. When using AMOS 24.0 (IBM SPSS) to verify a multiple mediator model, the total indirect effect can be verified, but each indirect effect cannot be verified. Therefore, in this study, a method of estimating the indirect effects of measurement variables using a virtual phantom variable was used. The method was proposed by Macho and Ledermann [48] and Cheung [52] to verify the indirect effect in a multiple mediator model by generating a phantom variable with a variance of 0. The phantom variables added to the research model are indicators of each indirect effect. The results regarding the mediating effects are presented in Table 7.
The analysis showed statistically significant indirect effects of interpersonal motive affecting subjective well-being by mediating online bonding (B=0.727, p<0.001), online bridging social capital (B=-0.409, p<0.05), and offline bonding social capital (B=-0.481, p<0.05). The indirect effects of interpersonal motive affecting subjective well-being through offline bridging social capital were not statistically significant. Meanwhile, the indirect effect of interpersonal motive on subjective well-being by dual-mediating online bonding social capital and offline bonding social capital was found to be statistically significant (B=0.097, p<0.05). Although the indirect effect of self-expression motive on subjective well-being through offline bonding social capital was statistically significant (B=0.148, p<0.05), the indirect effects of self-expression motive on subjective well-being through online bonding social capital, online bridging social capital, and offline bridging social capital were not statistically significant. The indirect effect of information-seeking motive on subjective well-being through offline bonding social capital was statistically significant (B=0.177, p<0.05), but it had indirect effects on subjective wellbeing through online bonding social capital, online bridging social capital, and offline bridging social capital, which were not statistically significant.
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to verify the effect of Social Media use motives on subjective well-being and to test the mediating effect of social capital on the relationship between Social Media use motives and subjective well-being.
First, an indirect effect of offline bonding social capital was found in the relationship between the interpersonal motive for Social Media use and subjective well-being. In particular, interpersonal motive was found to have a negative effect on offline bonding social capital, and decreased offline bonding capital negatively affected subjective well-being. This is the result of supporting the argument that interaction through the Internet has a negative effect on subjective well-being by reducing offline interaction [30,53]. Turkle [30] argued that media dependence such as excessive Social Media use can hinder true human relationships. The interpersonal motive for using Social Media can reduce intimate relationships such as family relationships or close friendships, and decreased offline bonding capital can lead to a decrease in subjective well-being.
Interestingly, the interpersonal motive was found to have a positive effect on subjective well-being through online bonding social capital and had a positive effect on subjective well-being by dual-mediating online and offline bonding social capital. Social Media can help remove barriers between users, thus enabling them to build new social capital, but it can also help maintain existing social relationships [7]. Bargh and McKenna [8] argued that an intimate relationship formed online can affect offline relationships, and Kim [9] said that reciprocal interactions obtained online can help solve offline relationship problems. In this context, it can be interpreted that reciprocal and reliable relationships acquired through Social Media can influence offline bonding social capital, which in turn can increase subjective well-being.
Second, there was no direct effect in the relationship between self-expression motive and subjective well-being, but an indirect effect through offline bonding social capital was observed. Self-expression through Social Media can increase social connectivity by increasing self-exposure and, consequently, may increase subjective well-being [22,54]. Third, although there was no direct effect in the relationship between information-seeking motive and subjective well-being, there was an indirect effect through the offline bonding social capital. In other words, the information-seeking motive for Social Media use can increase offline bonding social capital, thereby enhancing subjective well-being. This result is supported by previous studies, which found that information search activities have a positive effect on subjective well-being through social capital [55]. The information-seeking motive for Social Media use can help to share important information in daily life (e.g., health, economics) and help solve individual or daily problems, resulting in increased satisfaction in life.
The contributions and implications of this study are as follows. First, this study explained the specific mechanism by which Social Media use motives affect subjective well-being by mediating social capital. Second, past studies mainly focused on integrated social capital or offline social capital. However, with the development of the Internet, many people are forming social capital online [26]. Therefore, in this study, social capital was classified according to characteristics (bonding and bridging) and background (online and offline), and the specific relationships with Social Media use motives are verified.
The limitations of this study and suggestions for future studies are as follows. First, this study was conducted on Social Media users in their 20s, and there is a limitation that the results cannot be generalized to users of all ages. Second, because this study was designed as a cross-sectional study, it is important to be careful not to assert a causal relationship between each variable.
Notes
Availability of Data and Material
The datasets generated or analyzed during the study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: In-Pyo Hong, Sung-Man Bae. Data curation: In-Pyo Hong. Formal analysis: In-Pyo Hong. Investigation: In-Pyo Hong. Methodology: In-Pyo Hong, Sung-Man Bae. Project administration: In-Pyo Hong, Sung-Man Bae. Resources: In-Pyo Hong. Software: In-Pyo Hong. Supervision: Sung-Man Bae. Validation: Sung-Man Bae. Visualization: In-Pyo Hong. Writing—original draft: In-Pyo Hong. Writing—review & editing: Sung-Man Bae.
Funding Statement
None