Baduk (the Game of Go) Improved Cognitive Function and Brain Activity in Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Article information

Psychiatry Investig. 2014;11(2):143-151
Publication date (electronic) : 2014 April 11
doi : https://doi.org/10.4306/pi.2014.11.2.143
1Department of Psychiatry, YongSang Andong Hospital, Andong, Republic of Korea.
2Department of Psychiatry, Chung-Ang University Hospital, Seoul, Republic of Korea.
3Department of Psychiatry, Seoul National University College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea.
4Uimyung Research Institute for Neuroscience, Samyook University, Seoul, Republic of Korea.
5Laxtha Incorporated, Daejeon, Republic of Korea.
Correspondence: Doug Hyun Han, MD, PhD. Department of Psychiatry, Chung-Ang University Hospital, 102 Heukseok-ro, Dongjak-gu, Seoul 156-755, Republic of Korea. Tel: +82-2-6299-3132, Fax: +82-2-6299-1114, hduk@yahoo.com
Received 2013 May 06; Revised 2013 August 30; Accepted 2013 September 02.

Abstract

Objective

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms are associated with the deficit in executive functions. Playing Go involves many aspect of cognitive function and we hypothesized that it would be effective for children with ADHD.

Methods

Seventeen drug naïve children with ADHD and seventeen age and sex matched comparison subjects were participated. Participants played Go under the instructor's education for 2 hours/day, 5 days/week. Before and at the end of Go period, clinical symptoms, cognitive functions, and brain EEG were assessed with Dupaul's ADHD scale (ARS), Child depression inventory (CDI), digit span, the Children's Color Trails Test (CCTT), and 8-channel QEEG system (LXE3208, Laxtha Inc., Daejeon, Korea).

Results

There were significant improvements of ARS total score (z=2.93, p<0.01) and inattentive score (z=2.94, p<0.01) in children with ADHD. However, there was no significant change in hyperactivity score (z=1.33, p=0.18). There were improvement of digit total score (z=2.60, p<0.01; z=2.06, p=0.03), digit forward score (z=2.21, p=0.02; z=2.02, p=0.04) in both ADHD and healthy comparisons. In addition, ADHD children showed decreased time of CCTT-2 (z=2.21, p=0.03). The change of theta/beta right of prefrontal cortex during 16 weeks was greater in children with ADHD than in healthy comparisons (F=4.45, p=0.04). The change of right theta/beta in prefrontal cortex has a positive correlation with ARS-inattention score in children with ADHD (r=0.44, p=0.03).

Conclusion

We suggest that playing Go would be effective for children with ADHD by activating hypoarousal prefrontal function and enhancing executive function.

INTRODUCTION

ADHD and executive function

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurodevelopmental disorder in school-aged children, and has a worldwide prevalence rate of 5%.1,2,3 ADHD is characterized by age-inappropriate inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. These symptoms are assumed to result from disturbances in the brain region supporting executive function.4 Executive function is essential for cognitive development and is vital to individual social and intellectual function in daily life. Children with ADHD symptoms often show impairments in executive function, which has a significant impact on academic achievement and social functioning in daily life.5,6 One important neuropsychologic theory suggests that ADHD symptoms arise from a primary deficit in the executive functions that maintain the appropriate problem solving set for the attainment of later goals.7 Executive functions of perceptual cognition in relation to time and space, self-regulation, problem solving, logical and flexible thinking, and reconstitution of behavior have been suggested to be important in ADHD.7,8 Previous studies have also demonstrated that executive function can be enhanced by training intervention in children and adolescents with ADHD.9,10

ADHD treatment and adverse effects

Pharmacological treatment and behavioral interventions have been regarded as the standard treatment for ADHD.11 Stimulant medications are recommended as the first-line treatment for ADHD and can produce improvements in clinical and behavioral symptoms.12,13,14 However, non-pharmacological behavior modification treatments are also recommended for improving behavioral inhibition deficits and executive dysfunction in ADHD.15 For example, neurofeedback therapy, multimodal psychosocial treatment, school-based programs, working memory training, parent training, and self-monitoring have been introduced and applied as alternative treatments.10,16,17,18,19,20 EEG biofeedback, meditation, channel specific perceptual training, and vestibular stimulation have been applied for ADHD as alternative treatment with a promising prospective evidence.20 EMG biofeedback and cerebellar training demonstrated more evidences of treatment effectiveness.20 However, there is still a need for development of further adjuvant methods to improve ADHD symptoms and quality of daily life in children with ADHD.

Board games and executive function

Recent studies have demonstrated that board games involve many aspect of cognitive function. Several neuroimaging studies have revealed that the playing of board games such as Go or chess is related to executive function and problem solving skills.21,22 Chen et al. (2003) investigated the neural basis of Go using functional magnetic resonance imaging and observed enhanced activations in cortical areas of the dorsal prefrontal, parietal, occipital, posterior temporal, and primary somatosensory and motor areas. Quantitative analysis indicated a modest degree of stronger activation in the right parietal area than in the left, which are regions associated with executive function. Tachibana et al.23 investigated the efficacy of a Go intervention program for elementary school children.

The game of Go is a traditional board game for two players that originated in ancient China more than 2,500 years ago. The object of Go is to enclose a lager total area of the board than your opponent. The rules are simple, each player places black or white pieces alternately on the intersections of 19 by 19 crossed lines. The pieces may not be moved once placed on the board, although a player can capture and remove the opponent's pieces. Players must create at least two "eyes" in a group of stones to make them alive. If two eyes are present, the opponent can never capture a group of pieces.24,25 The key factor in playing Go is spatial positioning.

Playing Go requires several cognitive process steps related to executive function: attention, visuospatial perception, working memory, and decision making.26 Playing Go provides good opportunities to develop cognitive skills because of the many creative strategies available within a set of well-defined, simple rules.26 This game is noted for its rich strategy in spite of its simple rules. It was reported that long-term Go training appears to cause structural brain changes associated with higher-order cognitive capacities of learning, abstract reasoning, and self-control.27 Lee et al.27 conducted voxel-based analyses of diffusion-tensor imaging data and found that, compared to inexperienced controls, long-term trained Go players showed larger regions of white matter with increased fractional anisotropy values in the frontal, cingulum, and striato-thalamic areas, which are related to attentional control, working memory, executive regulation, and problem-solving. Additionally, the finding that fractional anisotropy is increased in the inferior temporal regions of expert Go players indicates that these players tend to develop a task specific template and right side dominance, suggesting that the tasks involved are mainly spatial processes.27 Long term Go training may cause structural brain changes associated with many cognitive properties, and such changes might be helpful for improving higher-order cognitive capacities such as learning, abstract reasoning, and self-control, which can facilitate education and cognitive therapies.26,27,28

Executive function and EEG changes in the prefrontal lobe

In a review of quantitative electroencephalography (QEEG) in children with ADHD, elevated theta power, reduced relative alpha and beta power and increased theta/beta ratio have been suggested as a marker of inattention and executive problems in ADHD.29,30,31 QEEG can serve as a valid neurometric tool in the diagnosis of ADHD,32 and changes in QEEG can now be used to evaluate changes in ADHD symptoms correlated with brain activity. Children with ADHD have been shown to have an altered pattern of QEEG activity compared with children without ADHD, especially during attention-loaded tasks, showing increased slow cortical activity mainly over in the frontal areas and decreased fast cortical activity.33 In addition, several stimulants and atomoxetine have been shown to change the EEG patterns in the frontal lobes of children with ADHD.34,35,36,37

We investigated the effectiveness of playing Go as a complementary therapy for improving executive function, including inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, in children with ADHD. We hypothesized that 16 weeks of Go training would change the clinical symptoms of ADHD, cognitive functions and the EEG pattern within prefrontal cortex in patients with ADHD. In details, ARS score and the time of Children's Color Trails Test (CCTT) would be decreased, the total score of digit span would be increased. In addition, theta/beta ratio within prefrontal cortex would be greatly decreased in ADHD group, compared to healthy control subjects.

METHODS

Subjects

We enrolled the participants by advertising at elementary schools in Andong. After obtaining informed consent from the children and their parents in accordance with Institutional Review Board guidelines, 42 participants were enrolled in this study. Two students with previous experience learning Go, one student with a tic disorder, and five students who dropped out prior to the start of the study were excluded. Seventeen drug-naïve children with ADHD were aged 7 to 12 years and seventeen age- and sex-matched comparison subjects without ADHD were recruited. These participants had no experience of other treatment for ADHD including behavioral therapy and psychosocial treatment. All children were screened with the Korean Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL) by psychiatrist and assessed with the ADHD Rating Scale (ARS).35,36,37,38 The ARS was performed by the parents of each child. There were no differences in the Child Depression Inventory scores between the ADHD and control groups (t=0.02, p=0.98) (Table 1).

Table 1

Comparison of demographic data and clinical characteristics

The inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) participants had no experience playing Go before the current study, 2) children in the ADHD group were diagnosed by a psychiatrist according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental disorder IV-TR criteria (DSM-IV-TR),37,38,39,40 3) children were able to attend all two hour/day, 5 days/week Go lessons during the 16-week study period. The exclusion criteria were as follows: 1) children who were being treated with stimulants, atomoxetine, neuroleptic, or any other psychoactive drugs or 2) children with organic brain disease, seizure disorder, mental retardation (IQ<70), severe illness or disorder, or other Axis I psychiatric diseases except ADHD, 3) those with any motor or perceptual handicap that would prevent them from playing Go.

Procedures

During the 16 weeks, both ADHD children without medication and children of control group were asked to learn and play Go for two hours/day with an instructor of the game of Go. Before and at the end of 16 weeks of playing Go, clinical symptoms, cognitive functions, and brain QEEG were assessed using ARS, CDI, the digit span forward/backward task, the CCTT, and 8-channel QEEG system (LXE3208, Laxtha Inc., Daejeon, Korea).

The children in the ADHD group had never taken medication for ADHD including stimulants, and had no other treatments for ADHD. The Go training curriculum was designed for beginners aged 7 to 12 and progressed for 16 weeks. Participants played Go under the instructor's education for 2 hours a day during weekday, Monday to Friday. Go training with the same protocol had been provided in two elementary schools and a public library in Andong, Republic of Korea as an after-school activity and the training curriculum was same.

Measures

Recording and analysis of quantitative electroencephalography

Electroencephalographic activity was recorded by QEEG-8 (LXE3208, Laxtha Inc., Daejeon, Korea). The electrodes were placed on Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, T3, T4, P3, P4 according to the International 10-20 System,41 with two additional electrodes placed on the inter-connected ear lobe to serve as the reference (A2) and ground (A1). The QEEG was recorded with a resolution of 12 bits, a low frequency filter of 0.5, a high frequency filter of 46 Hz, and at a sampling frequency of 256 Hz. Impedance was maintained below 5 kΩ.

All recordings were performed by the same EEG technician. Electroencephalographic activity was recorded with open eyes and during cognitive tasks for 5 minutes, during which participants were asked to judge whether or not pairs of figures were identical. If the pair of figures was identical, participants were asked to press the left arrow key on a computer keyboard, and if the pair of figures was not identical, participants were asked to press the right arrow key. The figures were modified from Raven's Progressive Matrices,42 which is often used as a nonverbal test in educational settings for groups ranging from 5 year olds to people who are elderly. In each test item, the subject is asked to identify the missing element that completes a pattern. This format is designed to measure reasoning ability or components of general intelligence.

Artifact-free 300-second periods were recorded and analyzed. Epochs of movement-related artifacts were excluded from the analyses by direct visual inspection of the raw data. The results were presented as absolute spectral power values (µV2) for individual segments of EEG spectrum theta (4-8 Hz), alpha (8-13 Hz), low beta (12-15 Hz), mid beta (15-20 Hz), and high beta (20-30 Hz). The average power spectrum of the EEG frequencies was calculated by the fast Fourier transform (FFT), a mathematical process that can be used to identify the various frequency bands (delta, theta, alpha and beta) on a QEEG. A Complexity 2.0 (Laxtha, Inc., Daejeon, Korea) EEG analyzing system was used for the analysis of electroencephalographic activity data.

Digit span forward/backwards test

The digit span task in the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children requires holding phonological units in the form of digits in short-term memory.43 The examiner reads aloud a sequence of numbers initially three digits in length and continues to nine digits numbers, and the participants are asked to repeat each sequence in the forward order and then in the reverse order. The digit span forward test determines verbal working memory and the digit span backwards test measures complex attention and working memory. Participants' scores were compared to age-matched normative data. Digit span internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha=0.90) and test-retest reliability (r=0.83, p<0.05) were both high.43,44,45

Children's Color Trails Test 1, 2

The CCTT was developed and standardized based on the Trail-Making Test as a Korean version for children.46 The Trail making test, an indicator of the speed of cognitive processing and executive functioning, is the most widely used instrument in neuropsychological assessment. The CCTT consists of two parts: CCTT-1 and CCTT-2. All data are presented as T-scores and are adjusted for age and sex. Higher T-scores indicate better performance on the test. The total time to finish the CCTT-1, CCTT-2 and the difference interference indices have been previously shown to differ significantly between normal children and children with untreated ADHD.47 CCTT-2 reflects primarily working memory, secondary task switching and reflects cognitive flexibility and cognitive persistence.48,49

DuPaul's ADHD Rating Scales

The severity of ADHD symptoms was assessed with the Korean version of the parent and teacher version of DuPaul's ADHD Rating Scale (ARS), which is outlined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental disorders.50 The Korean version of ARS has shown internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha=0.77-0.89) and has been reported to be highly valid and reliable.51 The ARS consists 18 items outlined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition criteria for diagnosing ADHD, nine of which are inattention (odd numbered) items and nine (even numbered) of which are hyperactive/impulsivity items. Each of the items has a 4-point scale scoring from 0 to 3 points (never or rarely, sometimes, often and very often). The ARS provides a total score, an inattention subscore, and a hyperactive/impulsive subscore. A higher score indicates a greater severity of ADHD. In this study, the ARS was performed by the parents of each child.

Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia-Present and Lifetime version-Korean version

The K-SADS-PL is an effective instrument for diagnosing major child psychiatric disorders. The validity and reliability of the Korean version was verified previously.38

Statistical analysis

The differences in demographic characteristics, ADHD symptoms, cognitive function, and QEEG between ADHD group and control group were assessed using the Mann-Whitney U Test. Differences in the changes in ARS total, inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity, digit span forward/backward, CCTT-2, and QEEG profiles during the 16 week Go training period were analyzed by repeated measures ANOVA. For correction of multiple comparisons, p values were set as less than 0.05/10 for EEG analysis. The effect size (f2) is 0.37 with 0.8 power (1-β). All analyses were performed using SPSS (version 11.0, IBM).

RESULTS

ADHD symptoms

During 16 weeks of learning Go, There were significant difference in the changes of ARS total score (F=23.1, p<0.01) and inattentive subscores of ARS (F=19.4, p<0.01) between ADHD group and healthy control group. In the ADHD group, the ARS total score (z=2.93, p<0.01) and the inattentive subscore of ARS (z=2.94, p<0.01) decreased over the 16-week Go training period (Figure 1). However, the hyperactivity subscore of ARS (z=1.33, p=0.18) did not change. In the control group, there were no changes in the ARS total score (z=1.03, p=0.30), inattentive scores (z=0.27, p=0.79), or hyperactivity scores (z=1.11, p=0.27) during the Go training period.

Figure 1

Changes in ARS score for the ADHD group compared with the control group. A: ARS-total score. B: ARS-inattention score. C: ARS-hyperactivity/impulsivity score. ARS: DuPaul's ADHD Rating Scale, ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, B: baseline, F: follow up.

Quantitative electroencephalography in prefrontal cortex

At baseline, relative high-beta right (Fp2), relative theta right (Fp2), theta/alpha left (Fp1), theta/alpha right (Fp2), theta/beta left (Fp1), and theta/beta right (Fp2) were higher in the ADHD group than in the control group. After 16 weeks of learning Go, the comparison subjects showed increased relative high-beta right (Fp2) (z=2.42, p=0.02), decreased relative theta left (Fp1) (z=2.43, p=0.02), relative theta right (Fp2) (z=2.42, p=0.02), theta/alpha left (Fp1) (z=3.88, p<0.01), theta/alpha right (Fp2) (z=3.88, p<0.01), theta/beta left (Fp1) (z=3.88, p<0.01), and theta/beta right (Fp2) (z=3.88, p<0.01), while children with ADHD showed decreased theta/alpha left (Fp1) (z=3.32, p<0.01), theta/alpha right (Fp2) (z=3.32, p<0.01), theta/beta left (Fp1) (z=3.32, p<0.01), and theta/beta right (Fp2) (z=3.33, p<0.01) (Figure 2). During 16 weeks of learning Go, the change in theta/beta right (Fp2) in children with ADHD was greater than that in children of comparisons (F=4.45, p=0.04) (Table 2). There were no significant different changes in other leads.

Figure 2

Comparisons of changes in the QEEG profile in the prefrontal cortex between the ADHD and control groups. A: Relative alpha left (Fp1) (F=2.16, p=0.15). B: Relative alpha right (Fp2) (F=2.44, p=0.13). C: Relative high-beta left (Fp1) (F=0.04, p=0.85). D: Relative highbeta right (Fp2) (F=0.54, p=0.47). E: Theta/alpha left (Fp1) (F=1.37, p=0.25). F: Theta/alpha right (Fp2) (F=1.38, p=0.24). G: Theta/beta left (Fp1) (F=3.86, p=0.06). H: Theta/beta right (Fp2) (F=4.45, p=0.04). ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, QEEG: quantitative electroencephalography, B: baseline, F: follow up.

Table 2

QEEG data of prefrontal cortex in the ADHD and control groups

Executive function

After the 16-week Go training period, the digit span total score (z=2.60, p<0.01) and digit span forward score (z=2.21, p=0.02) had increased in the ADHD group (Figure 3). The digit span backward score (z=1.80, p=0.07) also increased, but the increase was not statistically significant (Figure 3). The control group also showed improved digit span total scores (z=2.06, p=0.03) and digit span forward scores (z=2.02, p=0.04).

Figure 3

Comparisons of changes in executive function between the ADHD and control groups. Repeated measure ANOVA. A: Digit span total score (Dig-T) [Baseline (B)/Follow up (F)] (F=3.22, p=0.08). B: Digit span forward score (Dig-F) (F=2.08, p=0.16). C: Digit span backward score (Dig-B) (F=1.39, p=0.25). D: Children's Color Trails Test (CCTT) 2 (F=2.53, p=0.12). ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, ANOVA: analysis of variance.

ADHD children showed a decrease in CCTT-2 time (z=2.21, p=0.03) after the 16-week Go training period. However, there was no significant change in the comparison group. There were no significant statistical differences in digit total scores (F=3.22, p=0.08), digit forward scores (F=2.08, p=0.16), or digit backward scores (F=1.39, p=0.25) CCTT-2 (F=2.53, p=0.12) between the two groups (Table 3).

Table 3

Comparison of the changes in digit span and CCTT between the ADHD and control groups

Correlations between ARS score, cognitive symptoms, and EEG

The change in right theta/beta (Fp2) had a positive correlation with ARS-inattention scores and a negative correlation with digit span forward scores (r=-0.65, p=0.01) in children with ADHD (r=0.44, p=0.03).

DISCUSSION

Our current results suggested that playing Go may improve attention symptoms, executive function, and EEG changes in the prefrontal cortex in children with ADHD. In addition, changes in attention and executive function are associated with changes in EEG of the prefrontal cortex.

Go improved attention symptoms

We suggest that playing Go is effective for improving inattention in ADHD children. Previous studies have supported the suggestion that Go is effective for inattention. In a brain study of a Go player, a high degree of frontal lobe activation, and more specifically, the lateral prefrontal cortex, was observed. The activation of this area is associated with attention and spatial perception.26 The findings reported by Klingberg et al.10 provided preliminary evidence that visuospatial working memory training has therapeutic effects on ADHD symptoms, including inattention.

Go improved executive functions

The symptom of ADHD significantly correlated with executive functioning such as verbal and spatial working memory, attention shifting, sustained attention, cognitive inhibition, and visuospatial planning.21,26 The results of lower executive function in the ADHD group as compared with the control group as assessed with the digit span forward and the Children's Color Trails Test are in line with what previous studies have shown. ADHD is associated with significant weaknesses in several executive function domains, such as response inhibition, vigilance, working memory and planning.4 Barkely et al. (1997) suggested that ADHD symptoms arise from a primary deficit in executive functions, which are associated with the maintenance of a problem solving set appropriate for attainment of later goals.

After practicing Go for 16 weeks, notable improvements in executive function were observed in the ADHD group. The digit span forward results imply a positive effect on verbal working memory. The results of CCTT indicated improved visuoperceptual abilities (CCTT1) and cognitive flexibility and cognitive persistence (CCTT2),47,48,49 supporting the possible therapeutic effects of playing Go with ADHD children. Adaptive training of working memory (WM) has recently been shown efficacy as an alternative treatment for ADHD.9 It was demonstrated that computerized training of working memory improved response inhibition and reasoning and resulted in a reduction in parent-rated inattentive symptoms in children with ADHD.10

The change of EEG in prefrontal cortex

The theta/beta ratio (Fp1, Fp2) was higher in the ADHD group than in the control group, which is consistent with other studies of QEEG and ADHD.30,31 Various QEEG studies assessing different parameters have been carried out in children with ADHD, and the results have suggested a role for QEEG in evaluating symptom severity and distinguishing ADHD diagnostics.52 In previous studies comparing children with ADHD and children without ADHD, an increase in the delta and theta powers,53 increase in theta and decrease in beta54,55,56 and increase in theta and decrease in alpha and beta57 have been observed, which are characterized as a QEEG profile of ADHD.

Previous studies have shown increased theta activity and decreased beta activity in the prefrontal cortex of children with ADHD during cognitive tasks, indicating hypoarousal in the prefrontal cortex of children with ADHD related to a delay in maturation of cortical function.33 El-Sayed et al. demonstrated that an increase in the theta/beta ratio is a common trait in ADHD. After 16 weeks of learning and playing Go, the change in the theta/beta right in children with ADHD was greater than that in control group (F=4.45, p=0.04). Having more QEEG profile changes in right than the left prefrontal cortex was consistent with results of a previous study that suggested the presence of stronger activation in the right hemisphere than in left while playing Go.26 We suggest that Go training would be effective for ADHD children by activating hypoarousal of the prefrontal functions.

Changes in attention and executive function were associated with changes in the EEG of the prefrontal cortex. This result was consistent with previous studies showing the role of the prefrontal cortex in coordinating, controlling, and executing cognitive processes, allowing for the regulation of attention, impulses, and decision making.58,59

The current study had several limitations. First, the number of subjects was not large enough to generalize the effects of playing Go on children with ADHD. Future studies of a larger number of children will need to be done to conclusively investigate the effects of playing Go. Second, the EEG assessment was focused only on the prefrontal cortex.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study investigating the therapeutic effects of playing Go on enhancing cognitive functions in children with ADHD. We suggest that playing Go may be an alternative and complementary therapeutic method and that a modified intervention program for inattention and executive deficit based on the theory of playing Go could be developed.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant of the Korean Health Technology R&D Project, Ministry of Health & Welfare, Republic of Korea (A120013).

References

1. Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Epstein JN, Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK, Kahn RS. Prevalence, recognition, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a national sample of US children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2007;161:857–864. 17768285.
2. Patankar VC, Sangle JP, Shah HR, Dave M, Kamath RM. Neurological soft signs in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Indian J Psychiatry 2012;54:159–165. 22988324.
3. Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:942–948. 17541055.
4. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:1336–1346. 15950006.
5. Wu SY, Gau SS. Correlates for academic performance and school functioning among youths with and without persistent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Res Dev Disabil 2013;34:505–515. 23063730.
6. Yanez-Tellez G, Romero-Romero H, Rivera-Garcia L, Prieto-Corona B, Bernal-Hernandez J, Marosi-Holczberger E, et al. Cognitive and executive functions in ADHD. Actas Esp Psiquiatr 2012;40:293–298. 23165410.
7. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 1997;121:65–94. 9000892.
8. Shang CY, Wu YH, Gau SS, Tseng WY. Disturbed microstructural integrity of the frontostriatal fiber pathways and executive dysfunction in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychol Med 2013;43:1093–1107. 22894768.
9. Gibson BS, Gondoli DM, Johnson AC, Steeger CM, Dobrzenski BA, Morrissey RA. Component analysis of verbal versus spatial working memory training in adolescents with ADHD: a randomized, controlled trial. Child Neuropsychol 2011;17:546–563. 21390920.
10. Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlstrom K, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD - a randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2005;44:177–186. 15689731.
11. Spencer T, Biederman J, Wilens T, Harding M, O'Donnell D, Griffin S. Pharmacotherapy of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder across the life cycle. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;35:409–432. 8919704.
12. Hinshaw SP, Henker B, Whalen CK, Erhardt D, Dunnington RE Jr. Aggressive, prosocial, and nonsocial behavior in hyperactive boys: dose effects of methylphenidate in naturalistic settings. J Consult Clin Psychol 1989;57:636–643. 2794184.
13. Hechtman L, Greenfield B. Long-term use of stimulants in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: safety, efficacy, and long-term outcome. Paediatr Drugs 2003;5:787–794. 14658920.
14. Seixas M, Weiss M, Muller U. Systematic review of national and international guidelines on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Psychopharmacol 2012;26:753–765. 21948938.
15. Prasad V, Brogan E, Mulvaney C, Grainge M, Stanton W, Sayal K. How effective are drug treatments for children with ADHD at improving ontask behaviour and academic achievement in the school classroom? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2013;22:203–216. 23179416.
16. Duric NS, Assmus J, Gundersen D, Elgen IB. Neurofeedback for the treatment of children and adolescents with ADHD: a randomized and controlled clinical trial using parental reports. BMC Psychiatry 2012;12:107. 22877086.
17. Eiraldi RB, Mautone JA, Power TJ. Strategies for implementing evidence-based psychosocial interventions for children with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2012;21:145–159. 22137818.
18. Kang KD, Choi JW, Kang SG, Han DH. Sports therapy for attention, cognitions and sociality. Int J Sports Med 2011;32:953–959. 22068930.
19. Rutledge KJ, van den Bos W, McClure SM, Schweitzer JB. Training cognition in ADHD: current findings, borrowed concepts, and future directions. Neurotherapeutics 2012;9:542–558. 22911054.
20. Spetie L, Arnold EL. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactive Disorder. In : Martin A, Volkmar FR, Fred R, eds. Lewis's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: A Comprehensive Textbook 4th Editionth ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2007. p. 449–450.
21. Duan X, Liao W, Liang D, Qiu L, Gao Q, Liu C, et al. Large-scale brain networks in board game experts: insights from a domain-related task and task-free resting state. PLoS One 2012;7:e32532. 22427852.
22. Wan X, Nakatani H, Ueno K, Asamizuya T, Cheng K, Tanaka K. The neural basis of intuitive best next-move generation in board game experts. Science 2011;331:341–346. 21252348.
23. Tachibana Y, Yoshida J, Ichinomiya M, Nouchi R, Miyauchi C, Takeuchi H, et al. A GO intervention program for enhancing elementary school children's cognitive functions and control abilities of emotion and behavior: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 2012;13:8. 22239911.
24. Lin H. Introduction of Baduk (Traslated in Korean) Seoul: Samsin Press; 1970.
25. Sakata E. The Opening Theory of Go Seoul: Munwon Press; 1980.
26. Chen X, Zhang D, Zhang X, Li Z, Meng X, He S, et al. A functional MRI study of high-level cognition. II. The game of GO. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2003;16:32–37. 12589886.
27. Lee B, Park JY, Jung WH, Kim HS, Oh JS, Choi CH, et al. White matter neuroplastic changes in long-term trained players of the game of "Baduk" (GO): a voxel-based diffusion-tensor imaging study. Neuroimage 2010;52:9–19. 20394826.
28. An SG, Jeong SH, Baek KJ. A study on the effect of the brain activation and emotion by child Baduk study. J Technol Acad Indust Technol 2010;11:1436–1441.
29. Ogrim G, Kropotov J, Hestad K. The quantitative EEG theta/beta ratio in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and normal controls: sensitivity, specificity, and behavioral correlates. Psychiatry Res 2012;198:482–488. 22425468.
30. Quintana H, Snyder SM, Purnell W, Aponte C, Sita J. Comparison of a standard psychiatric evaluation to rating scales and EEG in the differential diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res 2007;152:211–222. 17451810.
31. Snyder SM, Quintana H, Sexson SB, Knott P, Haque AF, Reynolds DA. Blinded, multi-center validation of EEG and rating scales in identifying ADHD within a clinical sample. Psychiatry Res 2008;159:346–358. 18423617.
32. Snyder SM, Hall JR. A meta-analysis of quantitative EEG power associated with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Neurophysiol 2006;23:440–455. 17016156.
33. El-Sayed E, Larsson JO, Persson HE, Rydelius PA. Altered cortical activity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during attentional load task. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;41:811–819. 12108806.
34. Barry RJ, Clarke AR, Hajos M, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, Bruggemann JM. Acute atomoxetine effects on the EEG of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropharmacology 2009;57:702–707. 19698723.
35. Chabot RJ, Orgill AA, Crawford G, Harris MJ, Serfontein G. Behavioral and electrophysiologic predictors of treatment response to stimulants in children with attention disorders. J Child Neurol 1999;14:343–351. 10385840.
36. Song DH, Shin DW, Jon DI, Ha EH. Effects of methylphenidate on quantitative EEG of boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in continuous performance test. Yonsei Med J 2005;46:34–41. 15744803.
37. Swartwood MO, Swartwood JN, Lubar JF, Timmermann DL, Zimmerman AW, Muenchen RA. Methylphenidate effects on EEG, behavior, and performance in boys with ADHD. Pediatr Neurol 1998;18:244–250. 9568922.
38. Kim YS, Cheon KA, Kim BN, Chang SA, Yoo HJ, Kim JW, et al. The reliability and validity of Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia-Present and Lifetime Version-Korean version (K-SADSPL-K). Yonsei Med J 2004;45:81–89. 15004873.
39. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Editionth ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. Text Version.
40. Yang YH, Kim JW, Kim YN, Cho SC, Kim BN. Screening for Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder for children in Seoul. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc 2008;47:292–298.
41. American Electroencephalographic Society. Guideline thirteen: guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. J Clin Neurophysiol 1994;11:111–113. 8195414.
42. Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH. Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment; 2003.
43. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 4th Editionth ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2003.
44. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 3rd Editionth ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1991.
45. Gr ÉGoire J, Van Der Linden M. Effect of age on forward and backward digit spans. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 1997;4:140–149.
46. Shin MS, Koo HJ. Children's Color Trails Test Seoul: Hakjisa; 2007.
47. Koo HJ, Shin MS. A standardization study of Children's Color Trails Test. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008;:28–37.
48. Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2004;19:203–214. 15010086.
49. Sanchez-Cubillo I, Perianez JA, Adrover-Roig D, Rodriguez-Sanchez JM, Rios-Lago M, Tirapu J, et al. Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:438–450. 19402930.
50. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Editionth ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association Press; 1994.
51. So YK, Noh JS, Kim YS, Ko SG, Koh YJ. The reliability and validity of Korean Parent and Teacher ADHD Rating Scale. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc 2002;41:283–289.
52. Chabot RJ, di Michele F, Prichep L, John ER. The clinical role of computerized EEG in the evaluation and treatment of learning and attention disorders in children and adolescents. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001;13:171–186. 11449024.
53. Matsuura M, Okubo Y, Toru M, Kojima T, He Y, Hou Y, et al. A crossnational EEG study of children with emotional and behavioral problems: a WHO collaborative study in the Western Pacific Region. Biol Psychiatry 1993;34:59–65. 8373939.
54. Chabot RJ, Serfontein G. Quantitative electroencephalographic profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1996;40:951–963. 8915554.
55. Bresnahan SM, Anderson JW, Barry RJ. Age-related changes in quantitative EEG in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1999;46:1690–1697. 10624551.
56. Mann CA, Lubar JF, Zimmerman AW, Miller CA, Muenchen RA. Quantitative analysis of EEG in boys with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: controlled study with clinical implications. Pediatr Neurol 1992;8:30–36. 1558573.
57. Clarke AR, Barry RJ, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, Magee CA, Johnstone SJ, et al. Quantitative EEG in low-IQ children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 2006;117:1708–1714. 16793337.
58. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001;24:167–202. 11283309.
59. Benchenane K, Tiesinga PH, Battaglia FP. Oscillations in the prefrontal cortex: a gateway to memory and attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2011;21:475–485. 21429736.

Article information Continued

Funded by : Korean Health Technology R&D Project, Ministry of Health & Welfare
Award ID : A120013

Figure 1

Changes in ARS score for the ADHD group compared with the control group. A: ARS-total score. B: ARS-inattention score. C: ARS-hyperactivity/impulsivity score. ARS: DuPaul's ADHD Rating Scale, ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, B: baseline, F: follow up.

Figure 2

Comparisons of changes in the QEEG profile in the prefrontal cortex between the ADHD and control groups. A: Relative alpha left (Fp1) (F=2.16, p=0.15). B: Relative alpha right (Fp2) (F=2.44, p=0.13). C: Relative high-beta left (Fp1) (F=0.04, p=0.85). D: Relative highbeta right (Fp2) (F=0.54, p=0.47). E: Theta/alpha left (Fp1) (F=1.37, p=0.25). F: Theta/alpha right (Fp2) (F=1.38, p=0.24). G: Theta/beta left (Fp1) (F=3.86, p=0.06). H: Theta/beta right (Fp2) (F=4.45, p=0.04). ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, QEEG: quantitative electroencephalography, B: baseline, F: follow up.

Figure 3

Comparisons of changes in executive function between the ADHD and control groups. Repeated measure ANOVA. A: Digit span total score (Dig-T) [Baseline (B)/Follow up (F)] (F=3.22, p=0.08). B: Digit span forward score (Dig-F) (F=2.08, p=0.16). C: Digit span backward score (Dig-B) (F=1.39, p=0.25). D: Children's Color Trails Test (CCTT) 2 (F=2.53, p=0.12). ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, ANOVA: analysis of variance.

Table 1

Comparison of demographic data and clinical characteristics

Table 1

*p<0.05. CDI: Child Depression Inventory, CCTT1/2: Children's Color Trails Test 1/2, ADHD: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, ARS: ADHD Rating Scale

Table 2

QEEG data of prefrontal cortex in the ADHD and control groups

Table 2

*p<0.05. ADHD: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, QEEG: quantitative electroencephalography

Table 3

Comparison of the changes in digit span and CCTT between the ADHD and control groups

Table 3

CCTT: Children's Color Trails Test, ADHD: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder